Machiavellianism (psychology)

From WikiProjectMed
Jump to navigation Jump to search
Machiavellianism
Machiavellianism is one of the traits in the dark triad model, along with psychopathy and narcissism.
SpecialtyPersonality psychology
CausesGenetic and environmental[1][2]

In the field of personality psychology, Machiavellianism is a personality trait characterized by interpersonal manipulation, indifference to morality, lack of empathy, and a strategic focus on self-interest.[3][4][5][6] Psychologists Richard Christie and Florence Geis named the trait after Niccolò Machiavelli, as they used edited and truncated statements inspired by his works to study variations in human behaviors.[7][8][9] Their Mach IV test, a 20-question, Likert-scale personality survey, became the standard self-assessment tool and scale of the Machiavellianism construct. Those who score high on the scale (High Machs) are more likely to have a high level of manipulativeness, deceitfulness and a cynical, unemotional temperament.[10][11]

It is one of the dark triad traits, along with the subclinical versions of narcissism and psychopathy.[12][13][14]

Origin of the construct

Richard Christie was inspired by the works of Niccolò Machiavelli in studying the behaviors of those who manipulate others.

Inspiration from Machiavelli

In the early 16th century, Niccolò Machiavelli wrote several works on politics, including The Prince and The Discourses on Livy. These works were to bring Machiavelli everlasting fame, as they provided controversial statements regarding how monarchs should conduct themselves, such as deceiving enemies, and breaking pacts.[15] Although the word "Machiavellianism" originated in the late 16th century as a political term,[16] it would not become a concept in psychology until the 1960s, when Richard Christie and Florence L. Geis set out to study the thought processes and actions of those who manipulate others, such as political ideologues and religious extremists.[17] Christie was inspired by Machiavelli's writings, so he and his colleagues developed a test using a selection of statements, including truncated and edited sentences from Machiavelli's works as test items, naming the construct "Machiavellianism" after him.[18][4] They wanted to assess whether or not those who were in agreement with the statements would behave differently than others who disagreed, specifically in regards to manipulative actions. They published their results in their book, titled "Studies in Machiavellianism".

Though Christie named the trait after Machiavelli, he makes clear that he used sentences from Machiavelli's works only as a sort of litmus test to study deceptive and manipulative behavior, and that he was not concerned with their historical or philosophical significance, stating specifically that:[19]

Historians disagree as to whether Machiavelli was a cynic who wrote political satire, a patriot, or the first modern political scientist. The present concern is not with Machiavelli as an historic figure, but as the source of ideas about those who manipulate others.

Researcher and Psychology Today author Dale Hartley made this point more clear in his book, Machiavellians: Gulling The Rubes:[20]

When psychologists refer to the terms, Machiavellian or Machiavellianism, they do not mean Machiavelli's political advice. They refer instead to the underlying personality traits of cynicism, duplicity and scheming by which master manipulators conceive and execute their agendas.

Mach IV

Their Mach IV test, a 20-question, Likert-scale personality survey, became the standard self-assessment tool of the Machiavellianism construct.[8] Using their scale, Christie and Geis conducted multiple experimental tests that showed that the interpersonal strategies and behavior of "high Machs" and "low Machs" differ.[21] Those who score highly on the scale are classified as high Machs, while those who score low are classified as low Machs. People scoring high on the scale (high Machs) tend to endorse manipulative statements, and behave accordingly, contrary to those who score lowly (low Machs).[22] People scoring high on the scale (high Machs) tend to endorse statements such as, "Never tell anyone the real reason you did something unless it is useful to do so," (No. 1) but not ones like, "Most people are basically good and kind" (No. 4), "There is no excuse for lying to someone else" (No. 7) or "Most people who get ahead in the world lead clean, moral lives" (No. 11)[23] Their basic results have been widely replicated.[24] Measured on the Mach IV scale, males score, on average, slightly higher on Machiavellianism than females.[21][25]

The Mach IV test influenced the creation of an assessment called the Dirty Dozen, which contains 12 items, and the Short Dark Triad, composed of 27 items.[26]

High vs Low Machiavellianism

Like all personality traits, Machiavellianism is a characteristic that exists on a spectrum or continuum.[27][28] Christie and others noted that High Machs were more likely to cheat in experimental games, manipulate others around them, and were generally detached in their interactions. This was opposed to the behavior of Low Machs, who found such behaviors immoral, and refrained from them.[29]

The higher one is on the Machiavellianism scale, the more likely they are to deceive and exploit at the expense of someone else, engage in unprincipled behavior, and have a lack of empathetic feelings.[4]

Core features

In developing the construct studying manipulators, Christie theorized that they would possess the following characteristics:[30]

1. A relative lack of affect in interpersonal relationships: Manipulators do not empathize with their victims. The more empathy one has, Christie says, the less likely one will manipulate a person to do their bidding.

2. A lack of concern for conventional morality: Christie asserts that the manipulator is not concerned with the morality of behaviors such as lying and cheating.

3. A lack of gross psychopathology (mental illness): Christie states that manipulators usually have an instrumentalist view of the world, which shows a lack of psychosis or other mental impairments.

4. Low ideological commitment: Manipulators prefer to focus on getting things done pragmatically rather than focus on ideological allegiances. Christie states that while manipulators are to be found in organizations of differing ideals, they are more likely to be involved in tactics that achieve individual ends than inflexible idealistic ones.[31]

Five-Factor Model

Under the recently devised Five-Factor Model of Machiavellianism, three characteristics underlie the construct:[32]

  • Antagonism: manipulativeness, cynicism, selfishness, callousness, and arrogance.
  • Planfulness: deliberation and orderliness.
  • Agency: achievement-striving, assertiveness, self-confidence, emotional invulnerability, activity and competence.

Causes

Genetic and environmental

Several behavioral genetics studies on the dark triad have shown that Machiavellianism has both significantly genetic and environmental influences.[33][34][35][36] Researchers have noted that while Machiavellianism is heritable to a substantial degree, it can also be influenced by the shared-environment (i.e. sibling groups) slightly more than narcissism and psychopathy.[37][38][39] Other traits associated with machiavellianism are influenced by genetics as well, as one study notes that "The co-occurrence of alexithymia and Machiavellianism was most heavily influenced by genetic factors, and to a lesser but significant extent by non-shared environmental factors."[40] Machiavellianism is also heavily correlated with primary psychopathy which is itself strongly heritable.[41] A study on the "core" of dark triad traits also emphasized that the residual traits of Machiavellianism had "significant genetic components".[2] One particular study found a gene responsible for dopamine reception was positively associated with individuals who scored high on the MACH IV, but it is unclear what specific mechanisms cause this effect.[42][43]: 92  A study noted that the emotionality of Machiavellianism may also be genetically determined, with the authors stating that "it can be expected that in the case of Machiavellianism, the genetic influence may manifest itself by affecting the emotional sphere. That affective insensitivity – as McIlwain (2003) designates it--makes Machiavellians similar to psychopaths. In the study by Vernon et al (2008), genetic factors for Machiavellianism and psychopathy were correlated, which suggests that the variability of both qualities is greatly affected by the same genes. The genetically determined “cool syndrome” makes it easier for the child to use effective manipulation, leading to the development of a permanent behaviour strategy, at the same time protecting the child from internal punishments such as shame or guilt."[44]

Studies have found a link between Machiavellianism and a blunted cortisol awaking response (CAR).[45] A person's cortisol awaking response has been associated with a lack of affective empathy, and is highly heritable.[46][47]

Environmental causes

The environmental causes (such as shared, and non-shared environment) that contribute to the development of machiavellianism were childhood maltreatment and neglect, social reinforcement of manipulative behaviors from an early age, and poor family functioning.[37][48] One study even stated that "the etiology of Machiavellianism, similarly to the development of a dismissing-avoidant pattern, partly originates from childhood experiences obtained in relationships with unexpressive, less understanding, highly punitive or restrictive caregivers".[49][50][51] In many studies, Machiavellianism has been heavily correlated with negative home atmospheres, loneliness, and adverse parental experiences. One study noted that punishment specifically led to the "emergence of deceitful and exploitative interpersonal tactics. The authors of the study concluded that these results "might give further support for the idea that Machiavellian personality traits are possible strategic responses to childhood adversities".[48] Another study found the traits of Machiavellianism to be a response to early maladaptive schemas (EMS), essentially coping mechanisms for emotional deprivation, mistrust, abuse, and caregiver rejection.[52] Irregular parent care, such as a mother being neglectful and a father being overprotective, has also been correlated with Machiavellianism.[53] The hereditary influence on Machiavellianism may make it challenging to disentangle the genetic underpinnings from the effects of parental upbringing and environmental factors.[54]

Machiavellianism in children

Ever since the creation of the construct in the 1960s, there has been extensive research on Machiavellianism in young children and adolescents, via a measure dubbed the "Kiddie Mach" test.[55][56] The first study was done in 1966 as a part of Dorothea Braginsky's doctoral dissertation, with the subjects being as young as 10 years old.[57] Studies have shown that traits of Machiavellianism and other dark triad traits were already present in adolescents aged 11–17.[58] Deceptive behaviors by children even as young as 3 were also investigated heavily.[59][60][61] Peer reports suggest that children higher in Machiavellianism exhibit behaviors such as using both prosocial and coercive strategies based on how much is to be gained in a situation, and they tend to manipulate indirectly.[62] Children who score highly on the Machiavellianism scale tend to be more successful in manipulation, do it more frequently, and are judged as better at manipulation than those who score lower.[63] Parental levels of Machiavellianism seem to have a slight effect on the child's own level. Machiavellianism levels in fathers was positively correlated with the Machiavellianism levels of their children, but the mother's level had no significant effect. One study concluded that "parental Machiavellianism is a predictor and perhaps a cause of children's Machiavellian beliefs and their manipulative success".[64] Machiavellianism is also correlated with childhood aggression, especially concerning the control of social hierarchies.[65] One study found a trend upwards with respect to Machiavellianism from late childhood to adolescence, when levels of Machiavellianism are thought to peak. From adolescence throughout adulthood there is a significant and steady downward trend with regard to levels of Machiavellianism, until the age of 65 where an overall lifetime minimum is reached.[66]

Peer ratings of Machiavellian children are inconsistent, with some researchers reporting that Machiavellian children are rated as popular,[67] and some reporting that they are less well liked by peers.[68][69]

Dark triad

In 1998, John McHoskey, William Worzel, and Christopher Szyarto proposed that narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy are more or less interchangeable in normal samples.[70][71] Delroy L. Paulhus and McHoskey debated these perspectives at an American Psychological Association conference, inspiring a body of research that continues to grow in the published literature. Delroy Paulhus and Kevin Williams found enough differences between the traits to suggest that they were distinct despite their similarities, thus the concept of a "triad" of offensive personality traits was conceptualized.[72] There has been research on Machiavellianism using various dark triad measures, including the Short Dark Triad (SD3), and the Dark Triad Dirty Dozen test.[73][74]

Psychopathy

Many psychologists consider Machiavellianism to be essentially indistinguishable from psychopathy, as they both share manipulative tendencies and cold callousness as their primary attributes.[75][76] There is an immense and ongoing debate amongst researchers as to whether or not Machiavellianism and psychopathy should be treated as the same construct.[77][78] When tested, High Machs scored consistently high on measures of psychopathy, more than Low Machs.[79] According to John McHoskey, the MACH-IV test is merely "a global measure of psychopathy in noninstitutionalized populations".[80] Many have stated that the Machiavellianism scale measures nothing more than "successful" psychopathy, or psychopathy without the extreme clinical characteristics. [81] Even compared to other "dark" traits, research has shown that Psychopathy correlates with Machiavellianism far more than it correlates with narcissism.[76]

Researchers claim that Machiavellianism is well represented in Factor 1 of the Psychopathy Checklist, more specifically traits such as "conning/manipulative", "pathological lying,” and “glibness/superficial charm”.[82][83][84] Donald Lynam and others noted that Machiavellianism is represented even in the "Interpersonal Manipulation" factor in Hare’s Self-Report Psychopathy Scale-III and in the "Manipulation" scale in the Elemental Psychopathy Assessment. The researchers state that "ultimately, measures of psychopathy and MACH appear to be measuring the same construct, and MACH assessments fail to capture the construct as articulated in theoretical descriptions". [82] Both psychopaths and Machiavellians score low on conscientiousness and agreeableness, and they often are dismissive of social norms and ethics.[85] On the Psychopathic Personality Inventory, created by Scott Lilienfeld and Brian Andrews, there is even a subscale similarly named "Machiavellian Egocentricity".[86][87][88] The subscale is said to assess "ruthless attitudes in interpersonal functioning".[89] Machiavellianism and psychopathy have both been negatively correlated with the recognition of facial emotions, with one study stating that "if the ability to recognise facial expressions is exclusively an affective empathy task, it indeed accounts for the facial recognition deficits found in primary psychopathy and Machiavellianism."[90]

A recent study done on criminal psychopaths noted that Machiavellianism showed the strongest association with the affective aspect of psychopathy.[91]

Differences between constructs

Many other psychologists state that while Machiavellianism and psychopathy overlap heavily, there is much evidence to suggest that they are distinct personality constructs.[92][93][94] Psychologists who stress the differences between Machiavellianism and psychopathy state that, in total contrast to high Machs, psychopaths are impulsive, tend to be reckless, and lack long term planning skills.[95][96][97] Delroy Paulhus and others have stated that this difference between the two traits is often underappreciated.[98] Though both traits have a heritable basis, Machiavellianism is more influenced by the environment than psychopathy.[99] High Machs have been described as "master manipulators" and far better at manipulation than psychopaths and narcissists.[100]

Narcissism

Individuals high in Machiavellianism and narcissism both manipulate to improve their reputations, and how they appear to others.[101][102] Individuals high in the two traits do this as a form of self aggrandizement to help their chances of success in a given situation.[103] Machiavellianism scores were positively associated with aspects of narcissism such as entitlement and exploitativeness, and inversely associated with adaptive narcissistic tendencies, like self-sufficiency.[104] Studies have also shown that Machiavellians are more realistic about their character, while narcissists are less realistic about theirs.[105] Compared to High Machs, narcissists are less malevolent and show a more socially positive personality. They also have higher levels of self-rated happiness.[106]

While both Machiavellianism and narcissism involve a lack of empathy and a focus on self-interest, their motivations and manifestations differ. Machiavellians have tendencies to be driven by a desire for power and personal gain, whereas narcissists are driven by a need for validation and admiration.[107] Additionally, Machiavellians tend to be more strategic and calculating in their interpersonal interactions, while narcissists may be more impulsive and attention-seeking.[108]

In terms of social relationships, Machiavellians are more likely to exploit and manipulate others, while narcissists may be more likely to seek out relationships that serve to bolster their self-esteem and provide them with the admiration they desire.[109]

White collar crime

Research has shown that individuals high in Machiavellianism may be more willing to engage in white collar crimes.[110][111][112] Psychologist Daniel Jones has stated that "individuals higher on Machiavellianism are well suited for crimes in the financial world, especially crimes that skirt the legal system".[113] Delroy Paulhus has stated that Machiavellianism is the main trait for con artists, and not psychopathy, stating that:

Although direct research on this topic is difficult, it seems clear that malevolent stockbrokers such as Bernie Madoff do not qualify as psychopaths: They are corporate Machiavellians who use deliberate, strategic procedures for exploiting others. A genuine psychopath, even at the subclinical level, lacks the self-control to orchestrate the schemes of a shrewd stockbroker.[114]

In a research paper, Daniel Jones and others stated that a person high on Machiavellianism would also be possibly drawn to cybercrime, noting that "although we did not directly assess crimes, we did find patterns of system infiltration that were different among the three traits, with Machiavellianism being associated with the stealthiest approach among the three traits." They also stated that Machiavellianism would only be associated with crime if the "benefits outweighed the risks". They went on to clarify that this is opposed to individuals high in psychopathy, whom are prone to crime regardless of the situation.[115][116]

DSM: Trait, not disorder

Machiavellianism has never been considered a disorder, nor has it been referenced in any version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders.[117][118] It has been treated as strictly a personality construct. It is primarily studied by personality psychologists, as it is a non-clinical personality style.[119]

Relations with other personality traits

Big Five

Mach-IV scores are negatively correlated with agreeableness (r = −0.47) and conscientiousness (r = −0.34), two dimensions of the "big five" personality model (NEO-PI-R).[96] The FFMI corrects for this by including aspects of high conscientiousness in the scale (e.g. order, deliberation).[32] Additionally, Machiavellianism correlates more highly with the honesty-humility dimension of the six-factor HEXACO model than with any of the big five dimensions.[92] Machiavellianism has also been located within the interpersonal circumplex, which consists of the two independent dimensions of agency and communion.[120][121][122] Agency refers to the motivation to succeed and to individuate the self, whereas communion refers to the motivation to merge with others and to support group interests.[123] Machiavellianism lies in the quadrant of the circumplex defined by high agency and low communion.[92] Machiavellianism has been found to lie diagonally opposite from a circumplex construct called self-construal, a tendency to prefer communion over agency. This suggests that people high in Machiavellianism do not simply wish to achieve, they wish to do so at the expense of (or at least without regard to) others.[92][124]

Hot and cold empathy

There are two distinct types of empathy which people use to relate to each other which are referred to as hot and cold empathy.[125][126] Cold empathy (or cognitive empathy) refers to the understanding of how others might react to one's actions or a certain event.[127] Hot empathy (or emotional/affective empathy) refers to the emotional reaction others might have to the emotions of another person.[128][129] Machiavellianism was consistently negatively correlated with affective empathy in nearly every study.[130][131][132] Machiavellianism was also negatively correlated with affective resonance (feeling good when others feel good) and positively associated with affective dissonance (e.g. feeling happy when others are sad).[133] People high in Machiavellianism tend to have a better understanding of cold empathy and do not feel hot empathy which explains why they seem cold and uncaring.[134][135] Some studies have suggested that Machiavellians are deficient only at the level of affective empathy (sharing of emotions), whereas their cognitive empathy is intact, even high.[136] Another study suggested that high Machs are deficient at both kinds of empathy.[137] Studies also assert that high Machs do not feel guilt over the consequences of their manipulations.[138][139] High Machs are less likely to be altruistic, and they are less likely to be concerned with the problems of others.[140] One study proposed that High Machs have more automatic (that is, un-conscious) recognition of other's negative emotions more than low Machs, and that this understanding of emotions may in fact aid in the manipulation of others.[141]

Unemotionality

One of the primary traits of machiavellianism is a detached, unemotional attitude and lack of affect in regards to others.[142] Christie and Geis noted that the primary difference between high machs and low machs was the degree of emotion invested in interpersonal relations, with those scoring high having the lowest.[143][144] Research has been done on the extent of the low emotionality of those who score high on the Machiavellianism scale.[145] Doris Mcllwain noted that " Machiavellians do not inhabit the realm of emotion in the same way as others, yet they use it to manipulate others. They do not experience feelings, empathy, or morality in normative ways. yet they are consummate manipulators and deceivers precisely by playing upon these sentiments and convictions in others. Thus they induce in others the guilt they hardly feel themselves."[146] A study done by Farah Ali and others noted that Machiavellianism seems to have emotional reactions to stimuli similar to primary psychopathy, differing only in higher levels of anxiety, which those who score highly on machiavellianism may be prone to.[147][148] Machiavellianism has an unclear relation with anxiety levels, some researchers have found positive correlations, while some have found no relation at all.[149] Researchers have noted that High Machs may experience intense worries when things do not go their way, but are unable to express it.[150]

Alexithymia is also considered a key trait that is correlated heavily with Machiavellianism.[151][152][153] It is the lack of awareness of one's own emotions as well as the emotions of others.[154] When tested, healthy alexithymic individuals have been found to obtain high Machiavellianism scores.[155][152] This was not surprising to researchers, seeing as the unemotionality of Machiavellianism shows similarities to what alexithymics experience.[156] One study examined the relationship between alexithymia and endorsement of Machiavellian beliefs amongst university students. Results showed a positive correlation between alexithymia and Machiavellian beliefs, suggesting those with higher levels of alexithymia were more likely to endorse the view that manipulating others is an effective strategy. [157]

Motivation

A 1992 review described the motivation of those high on the Machiavellianism scale as related to cold selfishness and pure instrumentality, and those high on the trait were assumed to pursue their motives (e.g. sex, achievement, sociality) in duplicitous ways. More recent research on the motivations of high Machs compared to low Machs found that they gave high priority to money, power, and competition and relatively low priority to community building, self-love, and family commitment. High Machs admitted to focusing on unmitigated achievement and winning at any cost.[92][158][159]: 93 

The research on behaviors which high Machs engage in suggest that they are willing to achieve their goals by bending and breaking rules, cheating, and stealing.[160] People high in Machiavellianism are able to easily switch between working with others to taking advantage of others to achieve their goals, and they are more willing to do things others see as terrible or immoral.[134][161][162]

Mental abilities

Due to their skill at interpersonal manipulation, there has often been an assumption that high Machs possess superior intelligence, or ability to understand other people in social situations. Recent research provides some support for this assumption.[163] However, other research has established that Machiavellianism is unrelated to IQ.[164][165]

Furthermore, studies on emotional intelligence have found that high Machiavellianism is usually associated with low emotional intelligence as assessed by both performance and questionnaire measures.[166] Both emotional empathy and emotion recognition have been shown to have negative correlations with Machiavellianism.[167][168] Additionally, research has shown that Machiavellianism is unrelated to a more advanced theory of mind, that is, the ability to anticipate what others are thinking in social situations.[169] However, some studies have suggested the contrary viewpoint that high Machiavellianism is associated with excellent theory of mind skills.[170]

When it comes to manipulation, individuals high in Machiavellianism may, according to Bereczkei, "have certain cognitive and social skills that enable them to properly adapt to the challenges of environmental circumstances".[171] They also are incredibly perceptive to the presence of others, and are able to feign altruism to enhance their reputation.[172] A recent study investigated whether Machiavellianism is associated with the production of "bullshit" - inaccurate or meaningless information intended to impress, persuade or mislead. The researchers found that the manipulative aspect of Machiavellianism (Machiavellian approach) was linked to "persuasive bullshitting", aimed at gaining desired resources. The distrustful aspect (Machiavellian avoidance) was associated with "evasive bullshitting", spreading vague information to prevent disadvantages. Those high in Machiavellian avoidance were even better at distinguishing misinformation from valuable information.[173]

Social relationships

The effects that one's level of Machiavellianism has on a person's socialization and interpersonal relationships, such as friendships and romantic relationships, has been studied extensively. High Machs are highly likely to ingratiate themselves within social groups via compliments and conforming their opinions to those around them.[174] High Machs are more than likely to choose better quality friends, as they can guess better who is a good person and thus more pliable for manipulation.[175] Machiavellianism was also correlated with withdrawal and avoidance in romantic relationships.[176] Individuals high in all dark triad traits find it easy to end relationships, and tend to prefer short-term relationships over long term ones.[177][178] Studies done on courtship showed that women higher on machiavellianism tend to go on dates not for sexual reasons but for free food, a phenomenon known as a "foodie call".[179][180][181] Because a lack of empathy and affect with regards to others is one of the main features of Machiavellianism, individuals high on the trait tend to act in a utilitarian, self interested manner, prefer emotionally detached relationships, and are not concerned with the other person's needs.[182][183] High machs report lower relationship satisfaction than those lower on the scale.[184] Though there has been research on the potential "attractiveness" of the dark triad traits,[185] out of all of the traits in the dark triad, Machiavellianism was the least attractive to the opposite sex.[186][187][188] One of the studies concluded that "The third DT trait, Machiavellianism, was significantly negatively associated with being chosen and mate appeal for STR [short term relationships] in women."[187] Another study claimed that this was because high Machs tend to be way less extroverted than narcissists and psychopaths, and that "it is possible that individuals do not like cynical, manipulative, aggressive, remorseless, and duplicitous people such as Machiavellians and psychopaths".[188]

Like the other dark triad traits, those high on Machiavellianism may be more willing to troll or flame others on the internet, however a 2021 study found no particularly strong connection between trolling and dark personality traits.[189][190] Machiavellianism has also been correlated with a higher than average belief in conspiracy theories.[191][192]

Cross-cultural studies

There have been many studies on how Machiavellianism is presented in people from different countries and with different cultures. Multiple studies found that in nearly all countries, men scored higher than women in Machiavellianism, and that the gender differences were notable.[193] The populations of many other countries varied from their western counterparts in their levels of dark triad traits overall, which the authors of one study attributed to sociopolitical factors and levels of economic engagement.[194] In another cross cultural study, Machiavellianism also showed associations with limited interactive or normative values.[195] A study investigated the relationship between emotion recognition and dark personality traits (including Machiavellianism) across cultures. The effects were gender and culture-dependent. Among both German males and females, Machiavellianism showed strong positive associations with emotionally manipulative tactics.[196] Some scholars noted an issue with many cross cultural studies on machiavellianism, primarily that "researchers have used measures of Machiavellianism (Mach IV) which were derived from Western concepts and which may not have similar meanings when applied to non-Western groups."[197]

In the workplace

Machiavellianism is also studied by organizational psychologists, especially those who study manipulative behaviors in workplace settings.[198][199][200] Workplace behaviors associated with this concept include flattery, deceit, coercion, and the abuse of others through one's position of leadership.[201][202][203] These behaviors in the workplace are ultimately done to advance personal interests.[204][205][102] Research has shown that one's level of Machiavellianism can be a major factor in situations where workplace manipulation is involved because this trait can have an effect on the ability for an individual to "fit" into a highly political work environment.[206] Research has found individuals with Dark Triad traits are drawn to entrepreneurship. Certain qualities found in the Dark Triad are similar to traits needed for effective entrepreneurship, such as confidence, charisma and risk taking.[207][208]

Career advancement

Individuals high in machiavellianism tend to gravitate towards particular careers, especially those that require a high degree of competitiveness needed to succeed.[209] High Machs are ambitious enough to cut corners and use aggressive means if it is necessary to get ahead in their careers.[209] One study found that "Machiavellianism was positively related to leadership position and career satisfaction".[210] Individuals high in machiavellian traits are especially drawn to leadership and management positions, which became an important subject in the primary literature. Sales careers also attract dark triad individuals, with one study stating that such individuals are "prevalent" in the industry.[211] One study noted that those who possess machiavellian traits "are more productive but received lower overall managerial ratings", and that "Machiavellianism may in certain circumstances, be somewhat advantageous for long-term sales performance."[212] Machiavellianism was also associated with the use of "hard" (i.e. aggressive and hostile behavior) and "soft" (i.e. joking/kidding, offering compliments) tactics in the workforce.[213] One's political skill in the workplace and elsewhere may even mask the behaviors and characteristics associated with machiavellianism.[214]

It was shown that those high on Machiavellianism are more drawn to academic majors like economics, law, and politics, as opposed to the "person-oriented" majors like education, nursing, and social work that were associated with lower Machiavellianism scores.[215]

Dimensions of the MACH scale

Although there have been a myriad of proposed factor structures, two dimensions emerge most consistently within factor-analytic research – differentiating Machiavellian views from behaviors.[216] Although many posit that the Mach IV scale is unable to reliably capture the two dimensions, a 10-item subset of the scale known as the "two-dimensional Mach IV" (TDM-V), reproduces the views and tactics dimensions across countries, genders, sample types, and scale category length.[28][217] The "Views" dimension appears to capture the neurotic, narcissistic, pessimistic, and distrustful aspects of Machiavellianism, while the "Tactics" component captures the more unconscientious, self-serving, and deceitful behavioral aspects. More recently, in response to criticisms of the Mach-IV, researchers developed the Five-Factor Machiavellianism Inventory (FFMI), which attempts to include concepts (like being calculated and planful) that are not adequately captured by the Mach-IV.[32]

Most of the research done on Machiavellianism has been done with either the Mach IV or Mach V, though the Mach V is no longer in use due to psychometric issues.[216] Many have expressed concerns with the reliability of the Mach IV scale to capture all of the features of Machiavellianism, thus many proposals have been made in favor of other Machiavellianism scales.[218][219]

Scale Evaluation

Psychologist John Rauthmann and others have stated that, while the MACH-IV is "a generally reliable and valid scale", it has its shortcomings. These include the response styles of the test takers, the varying factor structures, and " insufficient content and construct validity". The researchers developed their own scale instead to study Machiavellianism multidimensionally instead of unidimensionally to prevent the construct from becoming hard to study effectively.[220] Psychologist Jason Dahling and others have created another measure of Machiavellianism, dubbed the Machiavellian Personality Scale (or MPS for short).[221]

Game theory

In 2002, the Machiavellianism scale of Christie and Geis was applied by behavioral game theorists Anna Gunnthorsdottir, Kevin McCabe and Vernon L. Smith in their search for explanations for the spread of observed behavior in experimental games, in particular individual choices which do not correspond to assumptions of material self-interest captured by the standard Nash equilibrium prediction.[25] It was found that in a trust game, those with high Mach-IV scores tended to follow Homo economicus' equilibrium strategies while those with low Mach-IV scores tended to deviate from the equilibrium, and instead made choices that reflected widely accepted moral standards and social preferences.[222]

A study done by David Wilson and other researchers noted that while High Machs tend to defect from their groups, they are also unlikely to succeed in the long term simply by manipulating others, and that some cooperation is necessary for further success and to avoid a situation in which they are retaliated against.[223][224]

See also

References

  1. ^ Vernon, Philip A.; Villani, Vanessa C.; Vickers, Leanne C.; Harris, Julie Aitken (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (2): 445–452. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007.
  2. ^ a b Schermer, Julie Aitken; Jones, Daniel N. (2020). "The behavioral genetics of the dark triad core versus unique trait components: A pilot study". Personality and Individual Differences. 154: 109701. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2019.109701. S2CID 210501437.
  3. ^ "APA Dictionary of Psychology".
  4. ^ a b c Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2009). Machiavellianism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 93–108). New York: The Guilford Press.[ISBN missing]
  5. ^ Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013-10-22). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. ISBN 978-1-4832-6060-0.
  6. ^ "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica". www.britannica.com. 2023-05-02. Retrieved 2023-06-21.
  7. ^ "Since most of the scale items and some of our research notions came from The Prince and The Discourses, it seems only candid to give credit where credit is due. Perhaps this book can be viewed as a partial vindication of Machiavelli's astuteness."
    -Studies in Machiavellianism, page 339
  8. ^ a b Colman, Andrew M. (2015). A Dictionary of Psychology - Andrew M. Colman - Google Books. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-965768-1.
  9. ^ Rauthmann, J. F., & Will, T. (2011). "Proposing a multidimensional Machiavellianism conceptualization". Social Behavior and Personality, 39(3), 391–404.
  10. ^ Spielberger, Charles D.; Butcher, James N. (2013). Advances in Personality Assessment. Routledge. ISBN 9781317844006.
  11. ^ Geis, F. L. (1978). “Machiavellianism,” in Dimensions of Personality, eds H. London and J. E. Exner Jr. (New York, NY: Wiley), 305–363
  12. ^ Paulhus, Delroy L; Williams, Kevin M (December 1, 2002). "The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6. S2CID 6535576 – via ScienceDirect.
  13. ^ Lyons, Minna (2019). The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-814292-9.
  14. ^ Furnham, Adrian; Richards, Steven C.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (March 10, 2013). "The Dark Triad of Personality: A 10 Year Review: Dark Triad of Personality". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1111/spc3.12018 – via CrossRef.
  15. ^ "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica". www.britannica.com. 2023-05-02. Retrieved 2023-06-23.
  16. ^ Giorgini, Giovanni (2013). "Five Hundred Years of Italian Scholarship on Machiavelli's Prince". Review of Politics. 75 (4): 625–640.
  17. ^ Studies in Machiavellianism, pages 2-6
  18. ^ Christie, Richard "On being detached about Machiavellianism"
  19. ^ Studies in Machiavellianism, pg. 1
  20. ^ Hartley, Dale (2 September 2023). Machiavellians: Gulling the Rubes. Draft2digital. ISBN 979-8-223-34675-3.
  21. ^ a b Christie, R. & Geis, F. (1970) Studies in Machiavellianism. New York: Academic Press. p. 260[ISBN missing]
  22. ^ "Meet the Machiavellians | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2022-09-14.
  23. ^ Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (2013). Studies in machiavellianism. Academic Press.
  24. ^ Repacholi, Betty; Slaughter, Virginia, eds. (March 2004). "Bypassing Empathy: A Machiavellian Theory of Mind and Sneaky Power". Individual Differences in Theory of Mind: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development. Psychology Press. pp. 40–67. doi:10.4324/9780203488508-7. ISBN 978-1-135-43234-8.
  25. ^ a b Gunnthorsdottir, Anna; McCabe, Kevin; Smith, Vernon (2002). "Using the Machiavellianism instrument to predict trustworthiness in a bargaining game". Journal of Economic Psychology. 23: 49–66. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00067-8.
  26. ^ Muris, Peter; Merckelbach, Harald; Otgaar, Henry; Meijer, Ewout (March 2017). "The Malevolent Side of Human Nature: A Meta-Analysis and Critical Review of the Literature on the Dark Triad (Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 12 (2): 183–204. doi:10.1177/1745691616666070. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 28346115. S2CID 24809359.
  27. ^ Katja Hutter, Johann Füller, Julia Hautz, Volker Bilgram & Kurt Matzler (2015) Machiavellianism or Morality: Which Behavior Pays Off In Online Innovation Contests?, Journal of Management Information Systems, 32:3, 197-228, DOI: 10.1080/07421222.2015.1099181
  28. ^ a b Monaghan, C., Bizumic, B., & Sellbom, M. (2016). The role of Machiavellian views and tactics in psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 72-81.
  29. ^ See Studies In Machiavellianism
  30. ^ Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013-10-22). Studies in Machiavellianism - Richard Christie, Florence L. Geis - Google Books. Academic Press. ISBN 9781483260600.
  31. ^ Studies in Machiavellianism, pg 3.
  32. ^ a b c Collison, K. L., Vize, C. E., Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2018). Development and preliminary validation of a five factor model measure of Machiavellianism. Psychological Assessment, 30(10), 1401–1407. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000637
  33. ^ Petrides, K. V.; Vernon, Philip A.; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Veselka, Livia (2011). "Trait Emotional Intelligence and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 14 (1): 35–41
  34. ^ Vernon, Philip A.; Villani, Vanessa C.; Vickers, Leanne C.; Harris, Julie Aitken (2008). "A behavioral genetic investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (2): 445–452. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.007.
  35. ^ Furnham, Adrian; Richards, Steven C.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (March 2013). "The Dark Triad of Personality: A 10 Year Review". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 7 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1111/spc3.12018. S2CID 32281294.
  36. ^ Veselka, L., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2011). Beyond the big five: The dark triad and the supernumerary personality inventory. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14(2), 158-168.
  37. ^ a b Monaghan, Conal, Boris Bizumic, and Martin Sellbom. "Nomological network of two-dimensional Machiavellianism." Personality and Individual Differences 130 (2018): 161-173.
  38. ^ Petrides, K. V., Vernon, P. A., Schermer, J. A., & Veselka, L. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence and the dark triad traits of personality. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14(1), 35-41.
  39. ^ "What Makes Someone a Master Manipulator? | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2023-06-24.
  40. ^ Cairncross, Molly; Veselka, Livia; Schermer, Julie Aitken; Vernon, Philip A. (June 2013). "A Behavioral Genetic Analysis of Alexithymia and the Dark Triad Traits of Personality". Twin Research and Human Genetics. 16 (3): 690–697. doi:10.1017/thg.2013.19. PMID 23561050. S2CID 40361747.
  41. ^ Bereczkei, Tamas (2015). "The manipulative skill: Cognitive devices and their neural correlates underlying Machiavellian's decision making". Brain and Cognition. 99: 24–31. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.06.007. PMID 26189112. S2CID 2671039.
  42. ^ "APA PsycNet".
  43. ^ Bereczkei, Tamás (12 December 2017). Machiavellianism: The Psychology of Manipulation. Routledge. ISBN 9781351605588.
  44. ^ Siwy-Hudowska, A., & Pilch, I. (2014). Machiavellianism in families: Analysis of relationships between the Machiavellianism level of grown-up children and their parents. Studia Psychologiczne, 52(3), 16-23.
  45. ^ Johnson, M. M., Caron, K. M., Mikolajewski, A. J., Shirtcliff, E. A., Eckel, L. A., & Taylor, J. (2014). Psychopathic traits, empathy, and aggression are differentially related to cortisol awakening response. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 36, 380-388.
  46. ^ Bartels, M., de Geus, E. C., Kirschbaum, C., Sluyter, F., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Heritability of Daytime Cortisol Levels in Children. Behavior Genetics, 33(4), 421–433. doi:10.1023/A:1025321609994.
  47. ^ Wüst, S., Federenko, I., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2000). Genetic factors, perceived chronic stress, and the free cortisol response to awakening. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25(7), 707–720. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(00)00021-4.
  48. ^ a b Láng, A. (2015). The relation between memories of childhood psychological maltreatment and Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 77, 81-85.
  49. ^ Ináncsi T, Láng A, Bereczkei T. Machiavellianism and Adult Attachment in General Interpersonal Relationships and Close Relationships. Eur J Psychol. 2015 Feb 27;11(1):139-54. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v11i1.801. PMID 27247647; PMCID: PMC4873099.
  50. ^ Jonason P. K., Lyons M., Bethell E. (2014). The making of Darth Vader: Parent-child care and the Dark Triad. Personality and Individual Differences, 67, 30–34. 10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.006
  51. ^ Láng, A., & Birkás, B. (2014). Machiavellianism and perceived family functioning in adolescence. Personality and individual differences, 63, 69-74.
  52. ^ Láng, A. (2015). Machiavellianism and early maladaptive schemas in adolescents.
  53. ^ Abell, L., Lyons, M., & Brewer, G. (2014). The relationship between parental bonding, Machiavellianism and adult friendship quality. Individual Differences Research, 12(4-B), 191–197.
  54. ^ https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s40750-016-0041-0
  55. ^ Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. p. 331. ISBN 9781483260600.
  56. ^ Chabrol, Henri; Van Leeuwen, Nikki; Rodgers, Rachel; Séjourné, Natalène (November 2009). "Contributions of psychopathic, narcissistic, Machiavellian, and sadistic personality traits to juvenile delinquency". Personality and Individual Differences. 47 (7): 734–739. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.020.
  57. ^ Studies in Machiavellianism, page 331
  58. ^ Lau, K. S. L., & Marsee, M. A. (2013). Exploring narcissism, psychopathy, and Machiavellianism in youth: Examination of associations with antisocial behavior and aggression. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22(3), 355–367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-012-9586-0
  59. ^ "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2023-06-26.
  60. ^ Lee, Kang (June 2013). "Little Liars: Development of Verbal Deception in Children". Child Development Perspectives. 7 (2): 91–96. doi:10.1111/cdep.12023. PMC 3653594. PMID 23687515.
  61. ^ Mitchell, Robert W.; Thompson, Nicholas S. (1986-01-01). Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit. State University of New York Press. ISBN 978-1-4384-1332-7.
  62. ^ Abell, Loren; Qualter, Pamela; Brewer, Gayle; Barlow, Alexandra; Stylianou, Maria; Henzi, Peter; Barrett, Louise (2015-08-20). "Why Machiavellianism Matters in Childhood: The Relationship Between Children's Machiavellian Traits and Their Peer Interactions in a Natural Setting". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 11 (3): 484–493. doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i3.957. ISSN 1841-0413. PMC 4873058. PMID 27247672.
  63. ^ Braginsky, Dorothea D. (1970). "Machiavellianism and manipulative interpersonal behavior in children". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 6: 77–99. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(70)90077-6.
  64. ^ "APA PsycNet".
  65. ^ Kerig, Patricia K.; Stellwagen, Kurt K. (2010). "Roles of Callous-Unemotional Traits, Narcissism, and Machiavellianism in Childhood Aggression". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 32 (3): 343–352. doi:10.1007/s10862-009-9168-7. S2CID 145468978.
  66. ^ Götz, Friedrich M.; Bleidorn, Wiebke; Rentfrow, Peter J. (October 2020). "Age differences in Machiavellianism across the life span: Evidence from a large-scale cross-sectional study". Journal of Personality. 88 (5): 978–992. doi:10.1111/jopy.12545. ISSN 0022-3506. PMID 32145085. S2CID 212629171.
  67. ^ Hawley P. H. (2003). Prosocial and coercive configurations of resource control in early adolescence: A case for the well-adapted Machiavellian. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 279–309. 10.1353/mpq.2003.0013
  68. ^ Palmen, H. (2009). Friendships and aggression in elementary school: The friendships of aggressive children and the effects of having aggressive friends (Doctoral thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands).
  69. ^ Abell, L.; Qualter, P.; Brewer, G.; Barlow, A.; Stylianou, M.; Henzi, P.; Barrett, L. (2015). "Why Machiavellianism Matters in Childhood: The Relationship Between Children's Machiavellian Traits and Their Peer Interactions in a Natural Setting". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 11 (3): 484–493. doi:10.5964/ejop.v11i3.957. PMC 4873058. PMID 27247672.
  70. ^ McHoskey, J. (1995). Narcissism and Machiavellianism. Psychological Reports, 77, 755–759.
  71. ^ McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 74, 192–210.
  72. ^ "The Light Triad vs. Dark Triad of Personality".
  73. ^ Jonason, P. K.; Webster, G. D. (2010). "The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad". Psychological Assessment. 22 (2): 420–432. doi:10.1037/a0019265. PMID 20528068.
  74. ^ Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (February 2014). "Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A Brief Measure of Dark Personality Traits". Assessment. 21 (1): 28–41. doi:10.1177/1073191113514105. PMID 24322012. S2CID 17524487.
  75. ^ As this is a recurring theme in the Machiavellianism literature, this list is by no means exhaustive:
  76. ^ a b Lee, K., Ashton, M., Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and Narcissism in the Five-Factor Model and the HEXACO model of personality structure.
  77. ^ Rauthmann, J. F., & Kolar, G. P. (2012). How “dark” are the Dark Triad traits? Examining the perceived darkness of narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(7), 884-889.
  78. ^ Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2015). Psychopathy and personality: Advances and debates. Journal of Personality, 83(6), 585-592.
  79. ^ Skinner, N. F. (1988). PERSONALITY CORRELATES OF MACHIAVELLIANISM: VI MACHIAVELLIANISM AND THE PSYCHOPATH. Social Behavior & Personality: an international journal, 16(1).
  80. ^ McHoskey, John W.; Worzel, William; Szyarto, Christopher (1998). "Machiavellianism and psychopathy". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (1): 192–210. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192. PMID 9457782.
  81. ^ Stellwagen, K. K. (Ed.). (2011). Psychopathy, narcissism, and Machiavellianism: Distinct yet intertwining personality constructs
  82. ^ a b Miller, J. D., Hyatt, C. S., Maples‐Keller, J. L., Carter, N. T., & Lynam, D. R. (2017). Psychopathy and Machiavellianism: A distinction without a difference?. Journal of personality, 85(4), 439-453.
  83. ^ McIlwain, D., Evans, J., Caldis, E., Cicchini, F., Aronstan, A., Wright, A., & Taylor, A. (2012). Strange moralities: Vicarious emotion and moral emotions in Machiavellian and psychopathic personality styles. Emotions, imagination, and moral reasoning, 119-148.
  84. ^ Skeem, J. L., Polaschek, D. L., Patrick, C. J., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic personality: Bridging the gap between scientific evidence and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12(3), 95-162.
  85. ^ Hartley, D.,Master Manipulators and Psychopaths Psychology Today.
  86. ^ "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2023-05-31.
  87. ^ Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Hicks, B. M., Blonigen, D. M., & Krueger, R. F. (2003). Factor structure of the psychopathic personality inventory: validity and implications for clinical assessment. Psychological assessment, 15(3), 340.
  88. ^ Kastner, Rebecca M., Martin Sellbom, and Scott O. Lilienfeld. "A comparison of the psychometric properties of the psychopathic personality inventory full-length and short-form versions." Psychological Assessment 24, no. 1 (2012): 261.
  89. ^ Poythress, Norman G., John F. Edens, and Scott O. Lilienfeld. "Criterion-related validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory in a prison sample." Psychological assessment 10, no. 4 (1998): 426.
  90. ^ Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality and individual differences, 52(7), 794-799.
  91. ^ "... Machiavellianism demonstrated the strongest association with the affective component of psychopathy." Kavish, N., Jones, M.A., Rock, R.C. et al. On the Overlap between Psychopathic Traits and Machiavellianism in a Forensic Population. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 41, 198–207 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-018-9708-0
  92. ^ a b c d e Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2009). "Chapter 7. Machiavellianism". In Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H (eds.). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. New York/London: The Guilford Press. pp. 257–273. ISBN 978-1-59385-647-2.
  93. ^ Paulhus, Delroy L.; Jones, Daniel N. (2015). "Measures of Dark Personalities". Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs. pp. 562–594. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00020-6. ISBN 978-0-12-386915-9.
  94. ^ Jones, Daniel N.; Mueller, Steven M. (2022-03-01). "Is Machiavellianism Dead or Dormant? The Perils of Researching a Secretive Construct". Journal of Business Ethics. 176 (3): 535–549. doi:10.1007/s10551-020-04708-w. ISSN 1573-0697. S2CID 231202731.
  95. ^ "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved 2022-09-14.
  96. ^ a b Paulhus, Delroy L.; Williams, Kevin M. (2002). "The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6. S2CID 6535576.
  97. ^ Bereczkei, Tamas "Machiavellianism: The Psychology of Manipulation" chap. 4
  98. ^ Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (February 2014). "Introducing the Short Dark Triad (SD3): A Brief Measure of Dark Personality Traits". Assessment. 21 (1): 28–41. doi:10.1177/1073191113514105. PMID 24322012. S2CID 17524487.
  99. ^ Siwy-Hudowska, A., & Pilch, I. (2014). Machia vellianism in families: Analysis of relationships between the Machiavellianism level of grown-up children and their parents. Studia Psychologiczne, 52(3), 16-23.
  100. ^ See the following:
  101. ^ Rauthmann, John F. (2011-09-01). "Acquisitive or protective self-presentation of dark personalities? Associations among the Dark Triad and self-monitoring". Personality and Individual Differences. Digit Ratio (2D:4D) and Individual Differences Research. 51 (4): 502–508. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.008. ISSN 0191-8869.
  102. ^ a b Lyons, Minna (2019-01-11). The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-814292-9.
  103. ^ Tracy, Jessica L.; Cheng, Joey T.; Robins, Richard W.; Trzesniewski, Kali H. (2009-04-01). "Authentic and Hubristic Pride: The Affective Core of Self-esteem and Narcissism". Self and Identity. 8 (2–3): 196–213. doi:10.1080/15298860802505053. ISSN 1529-8868. S2CID 18447793.
  104. ^ McHoskey, J. "Narcissism and Machiavellianism
  105. ^ Paulhus, D. L., Williams, K., & Harms, P. (2001). Shedding light on the dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy. In Annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology. San Antonio, TX.
  106. ^ Egan, Vincent, Stephanie Chan, and Gillian W. Shorter. "The Dark Triad, happiness and subjective well-being." Personality and individual differences 67 (2014): 17-22.
  107. ^ Zitek, E. M., & Jordan, A. H. (2019). Narcissism and motivation for power: A narcissistic admiration perspective. Journal of Research in Personality, 82, 103849.
  108. ^ Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011). The role of impulsivity in the Dark Triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(5), 679-682.
  109. ^ Czarna, A. Z., Jonason, P. K., Dufner, M., & Kossowska, M. (2016). The Dirty Dozen Scale: Validation of a Polish version and extension of the nomological net. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 445.
  110. ^ Konnikova, Maria (10 January 2017). The Confidence Game: Why We Fall for It . . . Every Time. Penguin. ISBN 9780143109877.
  111. ^ Tang, Thomas Li‐Ping, Yuh‐Jia Chen, and Toto Sutarso. "Bad apples in bad (business) barrels: The love of money, Machiavellianism, risk tolerance, and unethical behavior." Management Decision 46.2 (2008): 243-263.
  112. ^ Utami, I., Wijono, S., Noviyanti, S., & Mohamed, N. (2019). Fraud diamond, Machiavellianism and fraud intention. International Journal of Ethics and Systems.
  113. ^ Jones, D. N. (2016). The nature of Machiavellianism: Distinct patterns of misbehavior. In V. Zeigler-Hill & D. K. Marcus (Eds.), The dark side of personality: Science and practice in social, personality, and clinical psychology (pp. 87–107). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14854-005
  114. ^ Paulhus, Delroy L. (2014). "Toward a Taxonomy of Dark Personalities". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 23 (6): 421–426. doi:10.1177/0963721414547737. S2CID 44987860.
  115. ^ Jones, Daniel N., Edgar Padilla, Shelby R. Curtis, and Christopher Kiekintveld. "Network discovery and scanning strategies and the dark triad." Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021): 106799.
  116. ^ Hare, R. D. (1996). Psychopathy: A clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal justice and behavior, 23(1), 25-54.
  117. ^ "Psychiatry.org - DSM History".
  118. ^ "Machiavellianism | Definition, Politics, Psychology, & Facts | Britannica".
  119. ^ "MACH has historically been the province of personality and social psychology" McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 192–210.
  120. ^ Rauthmann, John F., and Gerald P. Kolar. "Positioning the Dark Triad in the interpersonal circumplex: The friendly-dominant narcissist, hostile-submissive Machiavellian, and hostile-dominant psychopath?." Personality and Individual Differences 54.5 (2013): 622-627.
  121. ^ McHoskey, John. "Narcissism and machiavellianism." Psychological reports 77.3 (1995): 755-759.
  122. ^ Jones, Daniel N., and Delroy L. Paulhus. "Differentiating the Dark Triad within the interpersonal circumplex." Handbook of interpersonal psychology: Theory, research, assessment, and therapeutic interventions (2010): 249-267.
  123. ^ Gurtman, Michael B. "Exploring personality with the interpersonal circumplex." Social and personality psychology compass 3.4 (2009): 601-619.
  124. ^ Jones, Daniel N.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (2012), "Differentiating the Dark Triad Within the Interpersonal Circumplex", Handbook of Interpersonal Psychology, John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, pp. 249–267, doi:10.1002/9781118001868.ch15, ISBN 9781118001868, S2CID 55425895
  125. ^ The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life pg. 16
  126. ^ Arefi, M. (2010). Present of a causal model for social function based on theory of mind with mediating of Machiavellian beliefs and hot empathy. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5, 694-697.
  127. ^ Ang, R. P., & Goh, D. H. (2010). Cyberbullying among adolescents: The role of affective and cognitive empathy, and gender. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 41, 387-397.
  128. ^ Cox, C. L., Uddin, L. Q., Di Martino, A., Castellanos, F. X., Milham, M. P., & Kelly, C. (2012). The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 7(6), 727-737.
  129. ^ https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8989800/
  130. ^ Massey-Abernathy, A., Byrd-Craven, J. Seeing but Not Feeling: Machiavellian Traits in Relation to Physiological Empathetic Responding and Life Experiences. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 2, 252–266 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-016-0041-0
  131. ^ The Dark Triad of Personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and Psychopathy in Everyday Life pg. 17-18
  132. ^ Blötner, Christian, Ricarda Steinmayr, and Sebastian Bergold. "Malicious mind readers? A meta-analysis on Machiavellianism and cognitive and affective empathy." Personality and Individual Differences 181 (2021): 111023.
  133. ^ Blötner, C., Steinmayr, R., & Bergold, S. (2021). Malicious mind readers? A meta-analysis on Machiavellianism and cognitive and affective empathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 181, 111023.
  134. ^ a b Taylor, Ben. Machiavellianism, Psych Central.
  135. ^ Paal, Tunde, and Tamas Bereczkei. "Adult theory of mind, cooperation, Machiavellianism: The effect of mindreading on social relations." Personality and individual differences 43.3 (2007): 541-551.
  136. ^
    • Al Aïn, Syrina; Carré, Arnaud; Fantini-Hauwel, Carole; Baudouin, Jean-Yves; Besche-Richard, Chrystel (2013). "What is the emotional core of the multidimensional Machiavellian personality trait?"
    • Barnett, M. A., and Thompson, S. (1985). The role of perspective-taking and empathy in children's Machiavellianism, prosocial behaviour, and motive for helping. J. Genet. Psychol. 146, 295–305. doi:10.1080/00221325.1985.9914459
    • Repacholi, B., Slaughter, V., Pritchard, M., and Gibbs, V. (2003). “Theory of mind, Machiavellism, and social functioning in childhood,” in Individual Differences in Theory of Mind. Macquarie Monographs in Cognitive Science, eds B. Repacholi, and V. Slaughter (Hove: Psychology Press), 99–120.
    • Richell, R. A., Mitchell, D. G., Newman, C., Leonard, A., Baron-Cohen, S., and Blair, R. J. (2003). Theory of mind and psychopathy: can psychopathic individuals read the language of the eyes? Neuropsychologia 41, 523–526. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00175-6
  137. ^ Lyons, M., Cadwell, T., and Shultz, S. (2010). Mind-reading and manipulation—is Machiavellianism related to theory of mind? J. Evol. Psychol. 8, 261–274. doi:10.1556/JEP.8.2010.3.7
  138. ^ McHoskey et al. (1998) Machiavellianism and Psychopathy
  139. ^ Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H. (17 December 2013). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. Guilford Publications. ISBN 9781462514892.
  140. ^ Kaufman, Scott Barry. "The Light Triad vs. Dark Triad of Personality". Scientific American Blog Network.
  141. ^ Bagozzi, R. P., Verbeke, W. J. M. I., Dietvorst, R. C., Belschak, F. D., van den Berg, W. E., & Rietdijk, W. J. R. (2013). Theory of mind and empathic explanations of Machiavellianism: A neuroscience perspective. Journal of Management, 39(7), 1760–1798.
  142. ^ Heym, N., Firth, J., Kibowski, F., Sumich, A., Egan, V., & Bloxsom, C. A. J. (2019). Empathy at the heart of darkness: Empathy deficits that bind the Dark Triad and those that mediate indirect relational aggression. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 95.
  143. ^ Wastell, C., & Booth, A. (2003). Machiavellianism: An alexithymic perspective. Journal of social and clinical psychology, 22(6), 730-744.
  144. ^ Studies in Machiavellianism, pg. 312
  145. ^ Jonason, Peter K., and Laura Krause. "The emotional deficits associated with the Dark Triad traits: Cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and alexithymia." Personality and Individual Differences 55.5 (2013): 532-537.
  146. ^ McIlwain, D. (2011). Young Machiavellians and the traces of shame: Coping with vulnerability to a toxic affect.
  147. ^ "Results indicated that primary psychopathy and Machiavellianism were positively associated with the experience of positive affect from sad stimuli, while secondary psychopathy and Machiavellianism were positively associated with the experience of negative affect in response to neutral stimuli"--- Ali, F., Amorim, I. S., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2009). Empathy deficits and trait emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and individual differences, 47(7), 758-762.
  148. ^ McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998). Machiavellianism and Psychopathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 192–210
  149. ^ Czibor, Andrea, Zsolt Peter Szabo, Daniel N. Jones, Andras Norbert Zsido, Tunde Paal, Linda Szijjarto, Jessica R. Carre, and Tamas Bereczkei. "Male and female face of Machiavellianism: Opportunism or anxiety?." Personality and Individual Differences 117 (2017): 221-229.
  150. ^ Szijjarto, L., & Bereczkei, T. (2015). The Machiavellians’“Cool Syndrome”: They experience intensive feelings but have difficulties in expressing their emotions. Current Psychology, 34, 363-375.
  151. ^ Wastell, Colin; Booth, Alexandra (December 2003). "Machiavellianism: An Alexithymic Perspective". Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 22 (6): 730–744. doi:10.1521/jscp.22.6.730.22931.
  152. ^ a b Al Aïn, Syrina; Carré, Arnaud; Fantini-Hauwel, Carole; Baudouin, Jean-Yves; Besche-Richard, Chrystel (2013). "What is the emotional core of the multidimensional Machiavellian personality trait?". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 454. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00454. PMC 3717508. PMID 23885245. Text was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 (CC BY 3.0) license.
  153. ^ "What Drives Master Manipulators | Psychology Today". www.psychologytoday.com. Retrieved 2022-09-15.
  154. ^ "Alexithymia: Symptoms, diagnosis, and links with mental health". www.medicalnewstoday.com. 25 September 2019. Retrieved 2020-04-29.
  155. ^ Simon, L. J., Francis, P. L., and Lombardo, J. P. (1990). Sex, sex-role, and Machiavellianism as correlates of decoding ability. Percept. Mot. Skills 71, 243–247. doi:10.2466/pms.1990.71.1.243
  156. ^ Geis, F. L. (1978). “Machiavellianism,” in Dimensions of Personality, eds H. London and J. E. Exner Jr. (New York, NY: Wiley), 305–363.
  157. ^ Sheikhi, S., Issazadegan, A., Norozy, M., & Saboory, E. (2017). Relationships between alexithymia and Machiavellian personality beliefs among university students. British Journal of Guidance & Counselling, 45(3), 297-304.
  158. ^ Spielberger, Charles D.; Butcher, James N. (2013). Advances in Personality Assessment. Routledge. ISBN 9781317844013.
  159. ^ Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (22 October 2013). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. ISBN 9781483260600.
  160. ^ Leary, Mark R.; Hoyle, Rick H. (2013-12-17). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. Guilford Publications. ISBN 978-1-4625-1489-2.
  161. ^ "APA PsycNet".
  162. ^ Rauthmann, John F. "Towards multifaceted Machiavellianism: Content, factorial, and construct validity of a German Machiavellianism Scale." Personality and Individual Differences 52.3 (2012): 345-351.
  163. ^ Kowalski, Christopher Marcin; Kwiatkowska, Katarzyna; Kwiatkowska, Maria Magdalena; Ponikiewska, Klaudia; Rogoza, Radosław; Schermer, Julie Aitken (2018). "The Dark Triad traits and intelligence: Machiavellians are bright, and narcissists and psychopaths are ordinary". Personality and Individual Differences. 135: 1–6. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.06.049. S2CID 149807368.
  164. ^ Jean M. Phillips; Stanley M. Gully (2013). Organizational Behavior: Tools for Success. Cengage Learning. pp. 85–. ISBN 978-1-133-95360-9.
  165. ^ Machiavellianism: A synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures. Wilson, David Sloan,Near, David,Miller, Ralph R. Psychological Bulletin, Vol 119(2), Mar 1996, 285-299
  166. ^ Austin, Elizabeth J.; Farrelly, Daniel; Black, Carolyn; Moore, Helen (2007). "Emotional intelligence, Machiavellianism and emotional manipulation: Does EI have a dark side?". Personality and Individual Differences. 43: 179–189. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.019.
  167. ^ Christie, Richard; Geis, Florence L. (2013). Studies in Machiavellianism. Academic Press. ISBN 9781483260600.
  168. ^ Rauthmann, John F.; Will, Theresa (30 April 2011). "Proposing a Multidimensional Machiavellianism Conceptualization". Social Behavior and Personality. 39 (3): 391–403. doi:10.2224/sbp.2011.39.3.391.
  169. ^ Lyons, M.; Caldwell, T.; Shultz, S. (2010-09-01). "Mind-reading and manipulation — Is Machiavellianism related to theory of mind?". Journal of Evolutionary Psychology. 8 (3): 261–274. doi:10.1556/jep.8.2010.3.7. ISSN 1789-2082.
  170. ^
  171. ^ Czibor, Andrea; Bereczkei, Tamas (2012). "Machiavellian people's success results from monitoring their partners". Personality and Individual Differences. 53 (3): 202–206. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.03.005.
  172. ^ "APA PsycNet".
  173. ^ Blötner, Christian, and Sebastian Bergold. "It is double pleasure to deceive the deceiver: Machiavellianism is associated with producing but not necessarily with falling for bullshit." British Journal of Social Psychology 62, no. 1 (2023): 467-485.
  174. ^ Pandey, Janak; Rastogi, Renu (August 1979). "Machiavellianism and Ingratiation". The Journal of Social Psychology. 108 (2): 221–225. doi:10.1080/00224545.1979.9711635. ISSN 0022-4545.
  175. ^ Jonason, P. K., & Schmitt, D. P. (2012). What Have You Done for Me Lately? Friendship-Selection in the Shadow of the Dark Triad Traits. Evolutionary Psychology, 10(3). https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491201000303
  176. ^ Brewer, Gayle; Parkinson, Madison; Pickles, Alice; Anson, Joshua; Mulinder, Georgia (2023). "Dark Triad traits and relationship dissolution". Personality and Individual Differences. 204: 112045. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2022.112045. S2CID 254775278.
  177. ^ Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., Webster, G. D., & Schmitt, D. P. (2009). The dark triad: Facilitating a short‐term mating strategy in men. European Journal of Personality: Published for the European Association of Personality Psychology, 23(1), 5-18.
  178. ^ Wilson, David Sloan, David C. Near, and Ralph R. Miller. "Individual differences in Machiavellianism as a mix of cooperative and exploitative strategies." Evolution and Human Behavior 19.3 (1998): 203-212.
  179. ^ Orhan, Mehmet A., et al. "Comparing Foodie Calls in Poland, the United Kingdom, and the United States: A Registered Replication Report." Psychological Reports (2023): 00332941231164079.
  180. ^ Collisson, Brian, Jennifer L. Howell, and Trista Harig. "Foodie calls: When women date men for a free meal (rather than a relationship)." Social psychological and personality science 11.3 (2020): 425-432
  181. ^ Deceptive Food Calls and the Dark Triad
  182. ^ Ali F., Chamorro-Premuzic T. (2010). The dark side of love and life satisfaction: Associations with intimate relationships, psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 228–233.
  183. ^ Studies in Machiavellianism, pg. 3, 93, 210
  184. ^ Brewer, G.; Abell, L. (2017). "Machiavellianism, Relationship Satisfaction, and Romantic Relationship Quality". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 13 (3): 491–502. doi:10.5964/ejop.v13i3.1217. PMC 5590532. PMID 28904597.
  185. ^ Carter, Gregory Louis, Anne C. Campbell, and Steven Muncer. "The dark triad personality: Attractiveness to women." Personality and Individual Differences 56 (2014): 57-61.
  186. ^ Rauthmann, John F.; Kolar, Gerald P. (2013). "The perceived attractiveness and traits of the Dark Triad: Narcissists are perceived as hot, Machiavellians and psychopaths not". Personality and Individual Differences. 54 (5): 582–586. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.005.
  187. ^ a b Jauk, Emanuel; Neubauer, Aljoscha C.; Mairunteregger, Thomas; Pemp, Stephanie; Sieber, Katharina P.; Rauthmann, John F. (2016). "How Alluring Are Dark Personalities? The Dark Triad and Attractiveness in Speed Dating". European Journal of Personality. 30 (2): 125–138. doi:10.1002/per.2040. S2CID 14542188.
  188. ^ a b Borráz-León, Javier I.; Rantala, Markus J. (2021). "Does the Dark Triad predict self-perceived attractiveness, mate value, and number of sexual partners both in men and women?". Personality and Individual Differences. 168: 110341. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2020.110341. S2CID 224989942.
  189. ^ Buckels, Erin E.; Trapnell, Paul D.; Paulhus, Delroy L. (September 2014). "Trolls just want to have fun". Personality and Individual Differences. 67: 97–102.
  190. ^ Brubaker, Pamela Jo; Montez, Daniel; Church, Scott Haden (April 2021). "The Power of Schadenfreude: Predicting Behaviors and Perceptions of Trolling Among Reddit Users". Social Media + Society. 7 (2): 205630512110213. doi:10.1177/20563051211021382. ISSN 2056-3051. S2CID 235724896.
  191. ^ March, E., & Springer, J. (2019). Belief in conspiracy theories: The predictive role of schizotypy, Machiavellianism, and primary psychopathy. PloS one, 14(12), e0225964.
  192. ^ Douglas KM, Sutton RM. Does it take one to know one? Endorsement of conspiracy theories is influ- enced by personal willingness to conspire.
  193. ^ Jonason, Peter K.; Foster, Joshua; Oshio, Atsushi; Sitnikova, Maria; Birkas, Bela; Gouveia, Valdiney (2017-07-15). "Self-construals and the Dark Triad traits in six countries". Personality and Individual Differences. 113: 120–124. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.053. ISSN 0191-8869.
  194. ^ Jonason, P. K., Żemojtel‐Piotrowska, M., Piotrowski, J., Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Gebauer, J. E., ... & Yahiiaev, I. (2020). Country‐level correlates of the dark triad traits in 49 countries. Journal of personality, 88(6), 1252-1267.
  195. ^ Jonason, Peter K.; Foster, Joshua D.; Kavanagh, Phillip S.; Gouveia, Valdiney V.; Birkás, Béla (October 2018). "Basic Values and the Dark Triad Traits". Journal of Individual Differences. 39 (4): 220–228. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000267. ISSN 1614-0001. S2CID 149501810.
  196. ^ Schmitt, Helena S.; Sindermann, Cornelia; Li, Mei; Ma, Yina; Kendrick, Keith M.; Becker, Benjamin; Montag, Christian (2020). "The Dark Side of Emotion Recognition – Evidence From Cross-Cultural Research in Germany and China". Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 1132. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01132. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 7363803. PMID 32733302.
  197. ^ Kuo, Hwang Kwang, and Anthony J. Marsella. "The meaning and measurement of Machiavellianism in Chinese and American college students." The Journal of Social Psychology 101, no. 2 (1977): 165-173.
  198. ^ "APA PsycNet". psycnet.apa.org.
  199. ^ James, Oliver (7 February 2013). Office Politics: How to Thrive in a World of Lying, Backstabbing and Dirty Tricks. Random House. ISBN 9781409005575.
  200. ^ Dugan, Riley; Rouziou, Maria; Hochstein, Bryan (December 2019). ""It is better to be loved than feared: Machiavellianism and the dark side of internal networking"". Marketing Letters. 30 (3–4): 261–274. doi:10.1007/s11002-019-09503-w. ISSN 0923-0645. S2CID 210553791.
  201. ^ Calhoon, R. P. (1 June 1969). "Niccolo Machiavelli and the Twentieth Century Administrator". Academy of Management Journal. 12 (2): 205–212. JSTOR 254816.
  202. ^ Kiazad, Kohyar; Restubog, Simon Lloyd D.; Zagenczyk, Thomas J.; Kiewitz, Christian; Tang, Robert L. (August 1, 2010). "In pursuit of power: The role of authoritarian leadership in the relationship between supervisors' Machiavellianism and subordinates' perceptions of abusive supervisory behavior". Journal of Research in Personality. 44 (4): 512–519. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.06.004 – via ScienceDirect.
  203. ^ Ellen, B. Parker; Alexander, Katherine C.; Mackey, Jeremy D.; McAllister, Charn P.; Carson, Jack E. (December 2021). "Portrait of a workplace deviant: A clearer picture of the Big Five and Dark Triad as predictors of workplace deviance". Journal of Applied Psychology. 106 (12): 1950–1961. doi:10.1037/apl0000880. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 33600196. S2CID 231964378.
  204. ^ No, a. i. (2018). “Cheating under pressure: A self-protection model of workplace cheating behavior”: Correction to mitchel et al. (2017). Journal of Applied Psychology, 103(1), 36. doi:10.1037/apl0000275
  205. ^ Shu, L. L., Gino, F., & Bazerman, M. H. (2011). "Dishonest deed, clear conscience: When cheating leads to moral disengagement and motivated forgetting". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 330–349 doi:10.1177/0146167211398138
  206. ^ Djurdjevic, Emilija; Rosen, Christopher C.; Conroy, Samantha A.; Rawski, Shannon L.; Sosna, Katherine U (June 2019). "The influence of political climate on job pursuit intentions and the moderating effect of Machiavellianism". International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 27 (2): 180–192. doi:10.1111/ijsa.12242. ISSN 0965-075X. S2CID 151216697.
  207. ^ Brownell, Katrina M.; McMullen, Jeffery S.; O'Boyle, Ernest H. (2021-05-01). "Fatal attraction: A systematic review and research agenda of the dark triad in entrepreneurship". Journal of Business Venturing. 36 (3): 106106. doi:10.1016/j.jbusvent.2021.106106. ISSN 0883-9026. S2CID 233542634.
  208. ^ Hmieleski, Keith M.; Lerner, Daniel A. (October 10, 2016). "The Dark Triad and Nascent Entrepreneurship: An Examination of Unproductive versus Productive Entrepreneurial Motives". Journal of Small Business Management. 54: 7–32. doi:10.1111/jsbm.12296. S2CID 152230559 – via CrossRef.
  209. ^ a b Gürlek, Mert (2021). "Shedding light on the relationships between Machiavellianism, career ambition, and unethical behavior intention". Ethics & Behavior. 31: 38–59. doi:10.1080/10508422.2020.1764846. S2CID 225719181.
  210. ^ Spurk, D., Keller, A. C., & Hirschi, A. (2016). Do bad guys get ahead or fall behind? Relationships of the dark triad of personality with objective and subjective career success. Social psychological and personality science, 7(2), 113-121.
  211. ^ Satornino, C. B., Allen, A., Shi, H., & Bolander, W. (2023). Understanding the Performance Effects of “Dark” Salesperson Traits: Machiavellianism, Narcissism, and Psychopathy. Journal of Marketing, 87(2), 298–318. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222429221113254
  212. ^ Ricks, J., Fraedrich, J. The Paradox of Machiavellianism: Machiavellianism May Make for Productive Sales but Poor Management Reviews. Journal of Business Ethics 20, 197–205 (1999). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005956311600
  213. ^ Jonason, P. K., Slomski, S., & Partyka, J. (2012). The Dark Triad at work: How toxic employees get their way. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(3), 449–453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.008
  214. ^ Blickle, Gerhard; Kückelhaus, Bastian P.; Kranefeld, Iris; Schütte, Nora; Genau, Hanna A.; Gansen-Ammann, Dominic-Nicolas; Wihler, Andreas (2020). "Political skill camouflages Machiavellianism: Career role performance and organizational misbehavior at short and long tenure". Journal of Vocational Behavior. 118. doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2020.103401. hdl:10871/122014. S2CID 214161864.
  215. ^ Gruda, D., McCleskey, J., & Khoury, I. (2023). Cause we are living in a Machiavellian world, and I am a Machiavellian major: Machiavellianism and academic major choice. Personality and Individual Differences, 205, 112096.
  216. ^ a b Fehr, B.; Samsom, D.; and Paulhus, D. L., (1992). "The Construct of Machiavellianism: Twenty Years Later". In C. D. Spielberger & J. N. Butcher (Eds), Advances in Personality Assessment (Vol 9), pp. 77–116. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  217. ^ Monaghan, Conal; Bizumic, Boris; Sellbom, Martin (2018). "Nomological network of two-dimensional Machiavellianism". Personality and Individual Differences. 130: 161–173. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2018.03.047. S2CID 150165701.
  218. ^ Rauthmann, J. F. (2013). Investigating the MACH–IV with item response theory and proposing the trimmed MACH. Journal of personality assessment, 95(4), 388-397.
  219. ^ Rauthmann, J. F., & Will, T. (2011). Proposing a multidimensional Machiavellianism conceptualization. Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal, 39(3), 391-403.
  220. ^ Rauthmann, John F., and Theresa Will. "Proposing a multidimensional Machiavellianism conceptualization." Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal 39.3 (2011): 391-403.
  221. ^ Dahling, Jason J.; Whitaker, Brian G.; Levy, Paul E. (2009). "The Development and Validation of a New Machiavellianism Scale". Journal of Management. 35 (2): 219–257. doi:10.1177/0149206308318618. S2CID 54937924.
  222. ^ Wörsdörfer, M. (2014). Inside the ‘Homo Oeconomicus Brain’: Towards a reform of the economics curriculum?. Journal of Business Ethics Education, 11, 1-36.
  223. ^ Wilson, David Sloan, David Near, and Ralph R. Miller. "Machiavellianism: a synthesis of the evolutionary and psychological literatures." Psychological bulletin 119, no. 2 (1996): 285.
  224. ^ Dahling, J. J., Whitaker, B. G., & Levy, P. E. (2009). The development and validation of a new Machiavellianism scale. Journal of management, 35(2), 219-257.

Further reading

  • Christie, R., & Geis, F. L. (1970). Studies in machiavellianism. Academic Press.
  • Jones, Daniel N., and Delroy L. Paulhus. "Machiavellianism." (2009).
  • Fehr, B., & Samsom, D. (1992). The construct of machiavellianism: tvventy years later. Advances in personality assessment, 9, 77.
  • Paulhus, Delroy L., and Kevin M. Williams. "The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism, and psychopathy." Journal of research in personality 36.6 (2002): 556-563.
  • Paulhus, Delroy L. "Toward a taxonomy of dark personalities." Current Directions in Psychological Science 23.6 (2014): 421-426.
  • Furnham, Adrian, Steven C. Richards, and Delroy L. Paulhus. "The Dark Triad of personality: A 10-year review." Social and personality psychology compass 7.3 (2013): 199-216.
  • Lyons, M. (2019). The dark triad of personality: Narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy in everyday life. Academic Press.
  • Bereczkei, Tamás. Machiavellianism: The psychology of manipulation. Routledge, 2017.
  • Hartley, D. (2023). Machiavellians: Gulling The Rubes.

External links